Skip to main content

Got a good feeling about someone? You probably just like the way they look.

[ad_1]


Ivan Pavlov had many research pursuits, but it’s his work with dogs that made him a household name. The Russian psychologist carried out unusual experiments on the behavior and gastric function of lovable mutts with names like Muyrka and Ukap. After years of work, he famously showed that one could use a relatively neutral stimulant (in this case, a bell) to trigger a reflexive response (canine salivation), a phenomenon now called classical conditioning.



But Pavlov’s dogs provided another important insight, too—one that’s had implications for almost every species on Earth, from pigeons to humans. New research suggests that when it comes to measuring the trustworthiness of strangers, we’re about as sophisticated as a few hungry puppers.



At the turn of the 20th century, Pavlov realized that conditioned dogs didn’t respond solely to the sound they were trained with. Rather, they could be encouraged to salivate with other stimuli, as long as that new stimulus shared some similarities with the original. In other words, if Pavlov played a collection of bells and each differed slightly from the one before, the dogs would still salivate. In keeping with this phenomenon, the dog's fervor would diminish slightly with each new bell, until they finally heard a bell so alien to them they didn’t respond at all. This symphony of saliva is called “stimulus generalization,” and it seems to play an important role in how we learn, too.



In a recent study in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, psychologists made the case for the role of stimulus generalization in how we interact with strangers. The researchers found that, in the absence of explicit information, people assess a stranger’s reputation based on how similar the newcomer looks to those they’ve met before.



Let’s say you meet a man named John. If you had friends in common, you might use their opinions to judge his moral character. (If someone says, “I know John and he’s a liar and a cheat,” for example, you might tread carefully.) But if you know nothing about him, this new study indicates, your brain uses unconscious clues to determine his trustworthiness. Specifically, you’ll unknowingly analyze John’s face and compare it to other faces you’ve seen before. If John looks, on the whole, like someone you know who you deem trustworthy, you and John could become fast friends. But if John looks like someone who’s crossed you before, the opposite could be true.



“In my field, there isn’t that much that’s known about how social learning unfolds,” says Oriel FeldmanHall, the lead author of the study. But this latest addition to the literature fills in a few gaps, showing the time-saving shortcuts we make to process our world. Stimulus generalization, FeldmanHall added, “is really adaptive and it makes learning much more efficient.”



To test this phenomenon, FeldmanHall and her colleagues couldn’t just stick a bunch of people in a room with actors who looked vaguely like their most hated family members. Instead, they conducted controlled experiments, using composites of human faces carefully crafted on a computer.



In the central experimental, study participants were asked to play a “trust game”—but it was a little different than the ones you remember from summer camp. Every player got $10, and researchers instructed them to fork over the figurative Hamilton to figures that appeared on the screen. Those simulated humans could then split the money with the original bill holder; the bigger the amount they gave back, the more trustworthy they were, more or less. Participants quickly learned which strangers were good and which were bad. In the next step, the study participants could pick the simulated stranger they wanted to play with. But the researchers had played a game of their own: Like Pavlov’s bells, all of the new partners were actually variations of the original crew.


Players were significantly more likely to play the trust game with the figures that looked like trustworthy partners from the previous round. Conversely, they were less likely to put their faith in facial morphs of the previously untrustworthy partners. All of this was operating at an unconscious level, like a knee-jerk reaction they didn’t even know they’d had.



While the researchers observed this nifty trick for self-education, FeldmanHall says they recognize it also has a dark side: Stimulus generalization could be closely related to implicit bias.



Implicit bias is a catchall phrase for the various stereotypes that unconsciously shape our understanding of the world and the people in it. Measuring the specifics of implicit bias is difficult, but research suggests jurors and judges are biased against black defendants compared to white defendants, and that doctors, hiring managers, and others may have implicit biases against non-white patients or potential hires.



It’s easy to see how this deep-seated Pavlovian learning tool could go awry in the human world, where we have a lot more on our minds than what’s for dinner (though dinner is extremely important). Many Americans live in homogenous communities, where they’re surrounded by people who look a lot like them. That can mess with your perceptions of people who aren’t like you. If the only diversity you see is on the news or in a TV show, your intuitions about strangers will probably be skewed, as you have very little—or perhaps just plain incorrect—information to guide you.



Implicit bias like this may be a sort of innate, subconscious response, just like a dog’s reaction to food. The study participants in FeldmanHall’s paper certainly didn’t know they were making decisions based on their past interactions with similar faces, just as the dogs weren’t purposefully slobbering all over themselves every time they heard that special sound. Fortunately for us humans, while we may share some hardwiring, we’re much more sophisticated than dogs; we don’t have to blindly accept every shortcut our neural circuits take.



They say you can’t unring a bell. That’s true, but you can certainly decide how you respond to the reverberations.




[ad_2]

Written By Eleanor Cummins

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Ice technicians are the secret stars of the Winter Olympics

[ad_1] The emphasis of this year's two-week-long Winter Olympic Games has been placed squarely on the Olympians themselves. After all, the stated purpose of the international competition is to bring together the world’s greatest athletes in a nail-biting competition across fifteen different winter sports. But before the curlers, skiers, and skaters even arrived in Pyeongchang, South Korea, the Olympians of the ice technician world were already a few weeks deep in a competition of their own. Mark Callan of the World Curling Federation and Markus Aschauer of the International Bobsleigh and Skeleton Federation both say they’re hoping to make the best ice the Winter Olympics have ever seen. To transform the barren concrete jungle of existing tracks and arenas into an ice- and snow-covered wonderland is an enormous undertaking. And it takes a keen understanding of the physics and chemistry that keeps frozen precipitation pristine. Curling Callan has been making and maintaining ic...

How to avoid the mid-movie bathroom break

[ad_1] Long movies and the urge to pee have been linked since the early days of cinema. Sixty-three years before Avengers: Endgame and its three-hour runtime, moviegoers settled in for nearly four hours of The Ten Commandments . “There will be an intermission,” director Cecil B. DeMille announced during the movie’s introduction. And audiences’ bladders were relieved. On average, movies aren’t getting longer, but they also don’t come with a predetermined bathroom break. That means when nature calls, you’ve got to either sit in growing discomfort or gamble on the best time to run to the restroom. But it doesn’t have to be this way, and for most people, setting your body to “do not disturb” is fairly simple. Go before the show The first piece of advice is also the easiest: pee before the movie starts. Generally, healthy adults urinate every 3-4 hours, so the longer a movie runs, the more urgent it becomes to reset your internal p...

Charted: Here's how much your food waste hurts the environment

[ad_1] Our species is pretty good at wasting food. Some we discard at the farm for being undersized or oddly shaped. Others we allow to decay in their shipping containers, thrown away before they even reach shelves. We leave even more foodstuffs wasting away in grocery stores, often by letting it sit there until it reaches its sell-by date. As consumers, we don’t have much control over most of the process that brings our food to the grocery store, but we do have control over how much food we personally waste. Let's face it: We’ve all found liquified lettuce in our veggie drawers. Don't fret. It's arguably impossible to consume 100 percent of the food we buy. But a healthy reminder of the effect food waste has on the environment might help us all to be more conscious of the amount of food we eat—and don't eat. Consumer food waste varies extensively depending on the area. In South and Southeast Asia, the Food and Agriculture Organization (FAO) estimates that only around ...